1092
Adults have learnt to make this distinction; they have also grasped the
uselessness of wishing, and after lengthy practice know how to postpone their
desires until they can find satisfaction by the long and roundabout path of altering
the external world. In their case, accordingly, wish-fulfilments along the
short psychical path are rare in sleep too; it is even possible, indeed, that they
never occur at all, and that anything that may seem to us to be constructed on
the pattern of a child’s dream in fact requires a far more complicated
solution. On the other hand, in the case of adults - and this no doubt applies without
exception to everyone in full possession of his senses - a differentiation has
occurred in the psychical material, which was not present in children. A
psychical agency has come into being, which, taught by experience of life, exercises
a dominating and inhibiting influence upon mental impulses and maintains that
influence with jealous severity, and which, owing to its relation to
consciousness and to voluntary movement, is armed with the strongest instruments of
psychical power. A portion of the impulses of childhood has been suppressed by this
agency as being useless to life, and any thought-material derived from those
impulses is in a state of repression.
Now while this agency, in which we recognize our normal ego, is concentrated
on the wish to sleep, it appears to be compelled by the psycho-physiological
conditions of sleep to relax the energy with which it is accustomed to hold down
the repressed material during the day. In itself, no doubt, this relaxation
does no harm; however much the suppressed impulses of the childish mind may
prance around, their access to consciousness is still difficult and their access to
movement is barred as the result of this same state of sleep. The danger of
sleep being disturbed by them must, however, be guarded against. We must in any
case suppose that even during deep sleep a certain amount of free attention is on
duty as a guard against sensory stimuli, and that this guard may sometimes
consider waking more advisable than a continuation of sleep. Otherwise there would
be no explanation of how it is that we can be woken up at any moment by
sensory stimuli of some particular quality. As the physiologist Burdach insisted long ago, a mother, for instance, will
be roused by the whimpering of her baby, or a miller if his mill comes to a
stop, or most people if they are called softly by their own name. Now the
attention which is thus on guard is also directed towards internal wishful stimuli
arising from the repressed material, and combines with them to form the dream
which, as a compromise, simultaneously satisfies both of the two agencies. The dream
provides a kind of psychical consummation for the wish that has been
suppressed (or formed with the help of repressed material) by representing it as
fulfilled; but it also satisfies the other agency by allowing sleep to continue. In
this respect our ego is ready to behave like a child; it gives credence to the
dream images, as though what it wanted to say was: ‘Yes, yes! you’re quite right,
but let me go on sleeping!’ The low estimate which we form of dreams when we
are awake, and which we relate to their confused and apparently illogical
character, is probably nothing other than the judgement passed by our sleeping ego
upon the repressed impulses, a judgement based, with better right, upon the motor
impotence of these disturbers of sleep. We are sometimes aware in our sleep of
this contemptuous judgement. If the content of a dream goes too far in
overstepping the censorship, we think: ‘After all, it’s only a dream!’ - and go on
sleeping.