4537
Probably no male human being is spared the fright of castration at the
sight of a female genital. Why some people become homosexual as a consequence of
that impression, while others fend it off by creating a fetish, and the great
majority surmount it, we are frankly not able to explain. It is possible that,
among all the factors at work, we do not yet know those which are decisive for the
rare pathological results. We must be content if we can explain what has
happened, and may for the present leave on one side the task of explaining why
something has not happened.
One would expect that the organs or objects chosen as substitutes for the
absent female phallus would be such as appear as symbols of the penis in other
connections as well. This may happen often enough, but is certainly not a
deciding factor. It seems rather that when the fetish is instituted some process
occurs which reminds one of the stopping of memory in traumatic amnesia. As in this
latter case, the subject’s interest comes to a halt half-way, as it were; it
is as though the last impression before the uncanny and traumatic one is
retained as a fetish. Thus the foot or shoe owes its preference as a fetish - or a
part of it - to the circumstance that the inquisitive boy peered at the woman’s
genitals from below, from her legs up; fur and velvet - as has long been
suspected - are a fixation of the sight of the pubic hair, which should have been
followed by the longed-for sight of the female member; pieces of underclothing,
which are so often chosen as a fetish, crystallize the moment of undressing, the
last moment in which the woman could still be regarded as phallic. But I do not
maintain that it is invariably possible to discover with certainty how the
fetish was determined.
An investigation of fetishism is strongly recommended to any one who still
doubts the existence of the castration complex or who can still believe that
fright at the sight of the female genital has some other ground - for instance,
that it is derived from a supposed recollection of the trauma of birth.
For me, the explanation of fetishism had another point of theoretical
interest as well. Recently, along quite speculative lines, I arrived at the
proposition that the essential difference between neurosis and psychosis was that in
the former the ego, in the service of reality, suppresses a piece of the id,
whereas in a psychosis it lets itself be induced by the id to detach itself from a
piece of reality. I returned to this theme once again later on.¹ But soon after
this I had reason to regret that I had ventured so far. In the analysis of two
young men I learned that each - one when he was two years old and the other
when he was ten - had failed to take cognizance of the death of his beloved
father - had ‘scotomized’ it - and yet neither of them had developed a psychosis.
Thus a piece of reality which was undoubtedly important had been disavowed by the
ego, just as the unwelcome fact of women’s castration is disavowed in
fetishists. I also began to suspect that similar occurrences in childhood are by no
means rare, and I believed that I had been guilty of an error in my
characterization of neurosis and psychosis. It is true that there was one way out of the
difficulty. My formula needed only to hold good where there was a higher degree of
differentiation in the psychical apparatus; things might be permissible to a
child which would entail severe injury to an adult.
¹ ‘Neurosis and Psychosis’ (1924b) and ‘The Loss of Reality in Neurosis and Psychosis’ (1924e).